Loading...
 
Toggle Health Problems and D

Omega-3 index of 6 to 7 associated with best cognition in this study – Nov 2019

Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids status and cognitive function in young women

Lipids in Health and Disease

VitaminDWiki

There are many studies of Omega-3 benefits to Cognition
This is the first study which found a decrease in cognition for the hightest Omega-3 Index levels
Note: In this obeservational study many women with low Omega-3 index were obese and had high CRP

Items in both categories Omega-3 and Cognitive are listed here:


Vitamin D and Omega-3 category starts with

394 Omega-3 items in category Omega-3 helps with: Autism (8 studies), Depression (29 studies), Cardiovascular (34 studies), Cognition (49 studies), Pregnancy (40 studies), Infant (32 studies), Obesity (13 studies), Mortality (7 studies), Breast Cancer (5 studies), Smoking, Sleep, Stroke, Longevity, Trauma (12 studies), Inflammation (18 studies), Multiple Sclerosis (9 studies), VIRUS (12 studies), etc
CIlck here for details

 Download the PDF from VitaminDWiki
Image
Image

Rebecca L. Cook1, Helen M. Parker1,2, Cheyne E. Donges3, Nicholas J. O'Dwyer1,3, Hoi Lun Cheng4,5, Katharine S. Steinbeck4,5, Eka P. Cox1, Janet L. Franklin6, Manohar L. Garg7 and Helen T. O'Connor1,2*

Background: Research indicates that low omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid (n-3 PUFA) may be associated with decreased cognitive function. This study examined the association between n-3 PUFA status and cognitive function in young Australian women.

Methods: This was a secondary outcome analysis of a cross-sectional study that recruited 300 healthy women (IB- 35 y) of normal weight (NW: BMI 18.5-24.9 kg/m2) or obese weight (OB: BMI >30.0 kg/m2). Participants completed a computer-based cognition testing battery (IntegNeuro™) evaluating the domains of impulsivity, attention, information processing, memory and executive function. The Omega-3 Index (O3I) was used to determine n-3 PUFA status (percentage of EPA (20:5n-3) plus DHA (22:6n3) in the red cell membrane) and the participants were divided into O3I tertile groups: T1 < 5.47%, T2 = 5.47-6.75%, T3 > 6.75%. Potential confounding factors of BMI, inflammatory status (C-reactive Protein), physical activity (total MET-min/wk), alphal-acid glycoprotein, serum ferritin and hemoglobin, were assessed. Data reported as z-scores (mean ± SD), analyses via ANOVA and ANCOVA.

Results: Two hundred ninety-nine women (26.9 ± 5.4 y) completed the study (O3I data, n = 288). The ANOVA showed no overall group differences but a significant group x cognition domain interaction (p <0.01). Post hoc tests showed that participants in the low O3I tertile group scored significantly lower on attention than the middle group (p = 0.01; ES = 0.45 [0.15-0.74]), while the difference with the high group was borderline significant (p = 0.052; ES = 0.38 [0.09-0.68]). After confounder adjustments, the low group had lower attention scores than both the middle (p = 0.01) and high (p = 0.048) groups. These findings were supported by univariate analyses which found significant group differences for the attention domain only (p = 0.004).

Conclusions: Cognitive function in the attention domain was lower in women with lower O3I, but still within normal range. This reduced but normal level of cognition potentially provides a lower baseline from which cognition would decline with age. Further investigation of individuals with low n-3 PUFA status is warranted.


Clipped from PDF

Results

The screening and recruitment of participants are summarized in Fig. 1. Although 299 women completed the study, blood samples could not be assayed in 11 cases, hence the analysis was based on 288 participants.
The demographic characteristics of the tertile groups, as well as their biochemical markers, are shown in Table 1. Years of education varied significantly across the groups (p = 0.02), the post hoc tests showing that the lowest O3I tertile group had about 1 year less education than the highest group (p = 0.02) while the difference from the middle group was not significant (p = 0.10). Of the five covariates, the O3I groups did not differ significantly on PA, SF or Hb, but there were significant group differences in BMI (and weight), CRP and a1GP levels. Post hoc pairwise comparisons showed that the lowest O3I group differed significantly from the highest group on these three variables (p < 0.02) but differed significantly from the middle group only on a1GP levels (p = 0.03). There were proportionately more participants with normal weight in the highest tertile group and more participants with obesity in the lowest tertile group (p < 0.0001).
The cognitive function of the three groups was assessed across the five domains. The mean z-scores for each domain across all O3I tertile groups were in the normal range (Fig. 2). The ANOVA showed no significant overall difference between the groups (p = 0.22) but there was a significant interaction between groups and cognitive domains (p <0.01). Examination of the mean values in Fig. 2 shows that the locus of this interaction was the attention domain, with the lowest tertile group having a lower score than the other two groups. Post hoc tests on this interaction confirmed that the lowest tertile group scored significantly lower on attention than the middle group (p = 0.01; ES = 0.45 [0.15-0.74]), while the difference with the highest group was borderline significant (p = 0.052; ES = 0.38 [0.09-0.68]). The middle and highest O3I groups had similar attention scores. Univariate analyses on each domain across the three groups confirmed a significant group effect only for attention (p = 0.004).
Of the five covariates, only BMI and CRP showed significant (weak) correlations with cognition domains, specifically attention and memory (r = - 0.14 to - 0.22, p < 0.02). BMI and CRP were also found to be positively correlated with each other (r = 0.67, p < 0.0001). Consequently, only the analyses of covariance with these two covariates had any effect relative to the unadjusted model and so only they will be reported. Similar to the unadjusted model, there were no significant overall group effects after adjusting individually for BMI (p = 0.28) and CRP (p = 0.23). The group x domain interaction was still significant for CRP (p = 0.013) but now borderline for BMI (p = 0.053), the locus again being the attention domain. Given these p values, post hoc tests were carried out in both cases and showed that for BMI, the lowest O3I group scored significantly lower on attention than both the middle (p = 0.01) and highest (p = 0.047) O3I groups, while for CRP, the lowest group scored significantly lower on attention than the middle group (p = 0.01) and was borderline significant with the highest group (p = 0.050). The middle and highest O3I groups again had similar performance scores. The univariate analyses on each domain across the three groups again confirmed significant effects for attention only (p < 0.029).
When these two covariates were combined in one ANCOVA model, the pattern of results mirrored that found after adjusting for BMI alone (the lack of further effect of CRP can be attributed to the degree of collin- earity between these covariates). There was no significant group effect (p = 0.27) and the group x domain interaction was again borderline (p = 0.054). Given this p value, post hoc pairwise comparisons were carried out and again showed that the lowest O3I group had significantly lower performance on attention than both the middle (p = 0.01) and highest (p = 0.048) O3I groups. The middle and highest O3I groups had similar attention scores. The univariate analyses were again significant for the attention domain (p = 0.029).
In summary, no overall group effect was observed with either the ANOVA or ANCOVA models but in each case, post hoc tests on significant group x domain interactions revealed a pattern whereby the lowest O3I tertile group scored significantly lower than the middle and highest tertile groups in the
Table 1 Demographics and biochemical markers (mean ±SD)

aOne-way ANOVA on all variables except Chi-square test for weight category distributions. Abbreviations: O3I omega-3 index, BMI body mass index, CRP C-reactive Protein, aiGP alpha1-acid glycoprotein, MET metabolic equivalent of task, min minute, wk. week, SF serum ferritin, Hb hemoglobin. Missing data: a1GP (n = 10),
Hb (n = 2)

cognitive domain of attention, with the latter groups showing similar scores. These findings were supported in each case by the univariate analyses, which found significant group differences for the attention domain (Fig. 2). When these ANOVA models were repeated on quartile (n = 72) and quintile (n = 57)

Fig. 2
O3I groups, a similar pattern of results was observed. There were no overall group effects but there were significant group x domain interactions (p < 0.02) on which post hoc tests revealed that the lowest quartile and quintile groups scored lowest in the domain of attention. Again, significant group differences in the attention domain were observed on univariate analyses (p < 0.01).

Discussion

This cross-sectional study examined the association of O3I with cognitive function in young, healthy, normal weight and obese women. While the cognitive performance of the participants was within the normal range, the study provides evidence for decreased performance in the attention domain in women with a lower Omega-3 Index. The major significant group differences remained after adjustment for known con- founders, with the post hoc tests still significant. Of the five confounders, BMI appeared to be most strongly linked to cognition. While a cut-off for n-3 PUFA intake for optimal brain health in young adults cannot be suggested at this stage, the lowest O3I tertile range (< 5.47%) may be sub-optimal for cognitive function. The reduced but normal level of cognition associated with lower n-3 PUFA potentially provides a lower baseline from which cognition would continue to decline with age.
There are a relatively small number of studies examining the effects of n-3 PUFA on cognitive function in young adults, ranging from randomized controlled trials to observational studies [1, 7, 38-45]. The majority of studies include psychometric tests measuring attention, memory and information processing, with memory tests more heavily represented in the literature. Overall, the evidence in healthy young adults suggests that dietary supplementation with n-3 PUFA does not enhance cognitive function, with only a handful of studies showing clear significant benefits on memory [40], information processing [44] and attention [43]. Importantly, none of the above-mentioned studies adjusted for confounders, which is a strength of our study. There is evidence however that a threshold effect of low n-3 PUFA on cognition may explain nonsignificant findings [38, 46, 47]; hence the effects of n- 3 PUFA supplementation in healthy participants may only be evident when cognitive performance is below average at baseline, or when n-3 PUFA status falls below a certain level (low or inadequate) [38]. Baseline or inadequate O3I levels has also been investigated in elderly cohorts, with O3I cut-off values suggested to define targets for future dementia trials. A study in dementia-free adults (70 years and over) reported an optimal cut-off of 5.3% for predicting notable cognitive decline and/or polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation treatment response [48]. This cut-off is close to that for the lowest O3I tertile group in the current study (< 5.47%).
Several mechanisms of action have been proposed to explain the relationship between n-3 PUFA and cognitive function. First, PUFAs are known to facilitate effects on gene expression, especially in the central nervous system [1, 50-52]. One genetic factor that is attracting increasing attention is the Apolipo- protein E (APOE) genotype, which has been linked to Alzheimer's disease. There is some evidence that this gene variant is also linked to cognitive performance in healthy young adults, but at present studies are conflicting and there is no consensus in the literature [49, 53, 54]. Secondly, it is known that n-3 PUFA has an important role in maintaining membrane integrity and fluidity, and neuronal functioning has been shown to be influenced by n-3 PUFA through a decrease in inflammatory pathways [1, 55, 56]. In support of the published reports, [57] C-reactive protein, an indicator of low-grade sustained inflammation, was lowest in women with the higher O3I in the present study, supporting a possible role of inflammation in determination of the attention domain of cognitive function.
Effects have also been found on dopaminergic neurotransmission, in particular, the mesocortical pathway has been implicated in attention, memory and executive function [56, 58, 59]. If these dopaminergic systems are altered via low n-3 PUFA, deficiency in this nutrient may contribute to reduced cognitive function. A recent study in diabetic rodents showed that administration of low doses of n-3 PUFAs could protect against neuronal damage in the hippocampus in type 2 diabetes and was associated with improved cognitive-behavioral performance and reduced inflammatory markers [55]. Furthermore, animal studies have reported evidence that n-3 PUFA (more specifically DHA) accumulates in areas of the brain involved in attention and memory, including the cerebral cortex and hippocampus [60, 61]. This observation is of interest, given that the current study found evidence of reduced cognitive function in the attention domain with lower O3I, albeit with no evidence of an effect on memory. There is limited information on the impact of short-term versus chronic inadequacy of n-3 PUFA or low O3I on cognition. However, in maternal and infant studies, there is evidence that there are crucial periods for adequate n-3 PUFA which may impact neurocognitive development [1, 3-5].
A major strength of this study is the recruitment of a large, healthy cohort, free of comorbidities. This study is one of the first to comprehensively exclude and/or adjust for a broad range of confounding variables when examining the influence of n-3 PUFA status on cognitive function in young women. Additionally, the use of erythrocyte n-3 PUFA (allowing for the calculation of the Omega-3 Index) provided an accurate and validated measure of longer-term n-3 PUFA status. Another major strength is the use of well-validated tools (IPAQ, Integ- Neuro™) to assess outcome measures.
A limitation of this study is that it is not possible to deduce causal effects due to the cross-sectional study design. Limitations more generally in the examination of the effect of n-3 PUFA on cognitive function include use of plasma or serum n-3 PUFA to classify status. The long-term measure of erythrocyte n-3 PUFA may be more precise but is less accessible. A major limitation in the literature, particularly in the studies conducted in young adults, is that confounder adjustment for factors that may potentially influence cognition, including inflammation, obesity and physical activity, is rarely conducted. Previous systematic reviews assessing the effect of n-3 PUFA supplementation on cognitive function have also cited large heterogeneity in supplement interventions (the type of supplement, EPA/DHA content, duration of intervention, etc.) and variability in cognition assessment, as major limitations in this field [3, 10, 42]. There is also currently no consensus regarding the classification of cognitive tests and domains [16] and this lack may explain some of the discrepancies in results between studies.

Conclusions

This study found reduced cognitive performance in the attention domain in young women with lower overall n- 3 PUFA, although cognition scores were still within the normal range. Thus, the clinical significance of these findings warrants further investigation. Cognition testing pre- and post-intervention to rectify low n-3 PUFA status and assessment of genetic factors (particularly APOE4 and dopamine receptor genes) may help to further identify the relationship and mechanisms of action between n-3 PUFA status and cognitive performance.

Abbreviations

ANCOVA: Analysis of covariance;
ANOVA: Analysis of variance;
APOE: Apolipoprotein E;
BMI: Body mass index;
CRP: C-reactive protein;
DHA: Docosahexaenoic acid;
EPA: Eicosapentaenoic acid;
ES: Effect size;
Hb: Hemoglobin;
IPAQ: International Physical Activity Questionnaire;
MET: Metabolic equivalent of the task;
n-3 PUFA: Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid;
NATA: National Association of Testing Authorities;
NW: Normal weight;
O3I: Omega-3 Index;
OB: Obese;
PA: Physical activity;
PUFA: Polyunsaturated fatty acid;
SD: Standard deviation;
SDT: Suggested dietary target;
SE: Standard error;
SF: Serum ferritin;
a1GP: Alpha1-acid glycoprotein

References

Junk

  1. Luchtman DW, Song C. Cognitive enhancement by omega-3 fatty acids from child-hood to old age: findings from animal and clinical studies. Neuropharmacology. 2013;64:550-65.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.019.
  2. Rangel-Huerta O, Gil A. Effect of omega-3 fatty acids on cognition: an updated systematic review of randomized clinical trials. Nutr Rev. 2018;76(1): 1 -20. https://doi.org/10.1093/nutrit/nux064.
  3. Jiao J, Li Q, Chu J, Zeng W, Yang M, Zhu S. Effect of n-3 PUFA supplementation on cognitive function throughout the life span from infancy to old age: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;100(6):1422-36. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.114.095315.
  4. Derbyshire E. Brain health across the lifespan: a systematic review on the role of omega-3 fatty acid supplements. Nutrients. 2018;10(8):1094. https:// doi.org/10.3390/nu10081094.
  5. McNamara RK, Asch RH, Lindquist DM, Krikorian R. Role of polyunsaturated fatty acids in human brain structure and function across the lifespan: an update on neuroimaging findings. Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2018;136:23- 34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plefa.2017.05.001.
  6. Meyer BJ. Are we consuming enough long chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids for optimal health? Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2011;85(5):275-80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plefa.2011.04.010.
  7. Jacka FN, Pasco JA, Williams LJ, Meyer BJ, Digger R, Berk M. Dietary intake of fish and PUFA, and clinical depressive and anxiety disorders in women. Br J Nutr. 2013;109(11):2059-66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114512004102.
  8. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Australian Health Survey: nutrition first results - food and nutrients, 2011-12. 2014. ABS cat no 4364.0.55.007. Available from: http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/abs@.nsf/DetailsPage/4364.0.55.0072 011-12?OpenDocument. [cited 2019 October 3]
  9. National Health and Medical Research Council. Nutrient reference values for Australia and New Zealand: fats: total fat and fatty acids. 2014. Available from: http://www.nrv.gov.au/nutrients/fats-total-fat-fatty-acids. [cited 2019 October 3]
  10. Cooper RE, Tye C, Kuntsi J, Vassos E, Asherson P. Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation and cognition: a systematic review and metaanalysis. J Psychopharmacol. 2015;29(7):753—63. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881115587958.
  11. Harris WS. The omega-3 index as a risk factor for coronary heart disease. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87(6):1997s-2002s. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/87.6.1997S.
  12. Fitzpatrick S, Gilbert S, Serpell L. Systematic review: are overweight and obese individuals impaired on behavioural tasks of executive functioning? Neuropsychol Rev. 2013;23(2):138—56. https://doi.org/10.007/s11065-013-9224-7.
  13. van den Berg E, Kloppenborg RP, Kessels RP, Kappelle LJ, Biessels GJ. Type 2 diabetes mellitus, hypertension, dyslipidemia and obesity: a systematic comparison of their impact on cognition. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2009; 1792(5)470-81. https://doi.org/10.1016Zj.bbadis.2008.09.004.
  14. Gimeno D, Marmot MG, Singh-Manoux A. Inflammatory markers and cognitive function in middle-aged adults: the Whitehall II study. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2008;33(10):1322-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.07.006.
  15. Luchsinger JA. Type 2 diabetes and cognitive impairment: linking mechanisms. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;30(Suppl 2):S185-S98. https://doi.org/10. 3233/JAD-2012-111433.
  16. Prickett C, Brennan L, Stolwyk R. Examining the relationship between obesity and cognitive function: a systematic literature review. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2015;9(2):93-113. https://doi.org/10.10167j.orcp.2014.05.001.
  17. Yang Y, Shields GS, Guo C, Liu Y. Executive function performance in obesity and overweight individuals: a meta-analysis and review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;84:225-44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.11.020.
  18. Pal K, Mukadam N, Petersen I, Cooper C. Mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia in people with diabetes, prediabetes and metabolic syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2018;53:1149. https//doi.org/10.1007/s00127-018-1581-3.
  19. StenforsCUD,Jonsdottir IH, Mangusson Hanosn LL, TheorellT. Associations between systemic pro-inflammatory markers, cognitive function and cognitive complaints in a population-based sample ofworking adults. J Psychosom Res. 2017;96:49-59. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjpsychores.2017.03.010.
  20. Cox EP, O'Dwyer N, Cook R, Vetter M, Cheng HL, Rooney K, et al. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive function in apparently healthy young to middle-aged adults: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(8):612-28. https://doi.org/10.1016/jjsams.2015.09.003.
  21. Ratey JJ, Loehr JE. The positive impact of physical activity on cognition during adulthood: a review of underlying mechanisms, evidence and recommendations. Rev Neurosci. 2011;22(2):171 —85. https://doi.org/10.1515/rns.2011.017.
  22. Sanders LMJ, Hortobagyi T, la Bastide-van Gemert S, van der Zee EA, van Heuvelen MJG. Dose-response relationship between exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2019;14(1):e0210036. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0210036.
  23. Northey JM, Cherbuin N, Pumpa KL, Smee DJ, Rattray B. Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2018;52:154-60. https://doi.org/ 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587.
  24. Vazou S, Pesce C, Lakes K, Smiley-Oyen A. More than one road leads to Rome: a narrative review and meta-analysis of physical activity intervention effects on cognition in youth. Int J Sport Exer Psychol. 2019;17(2):153-78. https://doi.org/10.1080/1612197X.2016.1223423.
  25. Cook RL, O'Dwyer NJ, Parker HM, Donges CE, Cheng HL, Steinbeck KS, et al. Iron deficiency anaemia, not iron deficiency, is associated with reduced attention in healthy young women. Nutrients. 2017;9(11):1216. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu9111216.
  26. Greig AJ, Patterson AJ, Collins CE, Chalmers KA. Iron deficiency, cognition, mental health amd fatigue in women of childbearing age: a systematic review. J Nutr Sci. 2013;2:e14 doi: 10/1.1017/jns.2013.7.
  27. Australian Bureau of Statistics. National Health Survey: first results 2017-2018 overweight and obesity. 2019. ABS cat. No. 4364.0.55.001. Available from: https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/Lookup/by%20Subject/4364.0.55. 001~2017-18~Main%20Features~Overweight%20and%20obesity~90. [cited 2019 October 3]
  28. Mishra G, Loxton D, Anderson A, Hockey R, Powers J, Brown W, et al. Health and well-being of women aged 18 to 23 in 2003 and 1996: findings from the Australian longitudinal study on women's health. 2014. Available from: https://www.alswh.org.au/images/content/pdf/major_reports/2014Major_ Report%20I_FINAL.pdf. [cited 2019 October 3]
  29. Australian Bureau of Statistics. National Health Survey: first results 2017-2018 physical activity. 2019. ABS cat. No. 4363.0.55.001. Available from:https:// www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/Lookup/by%20Subject/4364.0.55.001~201 7-18~Main%20Features~Physical%20activity~115. [cited 2019 October 3]
  30. World Health Organisation. Obesity and overweight. 2019. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight. [cited 2019 October 3]
  31. Paul RH, Lawrence J, Williams LM, Clark RC, Cooper N, Gordon E. Preliminary validity of "integneuroTM": a new computerized battery of neurocognitive tests. J Neuroscience. 2005;115(11):1549-67. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207450590957890.
  32. Northrop-Clewes CA, Thurnham DI. Biomarkers for the differentiation of anemia and their clinical usefulness. J Blood Med. 2013;4:11 -22. https://doi.org/10.2147/JBM.S29212.
  33. Thurnham DI, McCabe LD, Haldar S, Wieringa FT, Northrop-Clewes CA, McCabe GP. Adjusting plasma ferritin concentrations to remove the effects of subclinical inflammation in the assessment of iron deficiency: a meta-analysis. Am J Clin Nutr. 2010;92(3):546-55. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2010.29284.
  34. Ferguson JJ, Veysey M, Lucock M, Niblett S, King K, MacDonald-Wicks L, et al. Association between omega-3 index and blood lipids in older Australians. J Nutr Biochem. 2016;27:233-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/). jnutbio.2015.09.010.
  35. Harris WS, Von Schacky C. The omega-3 index: a new risk factor for death from coronary heart disease? Prev Med. 2004;39(1):212-20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.02.030.
  36. Ekelund U, Sepp H, Brage S, Becker W, Jakes R, Hennings M, et al. Criterion- related validity of the last 7-day, short form of the international physical activity questionnaire in Swedish adults. Public Health Nutr. 2006;9(2):258- 65. https://doi.org/10.1079/PHN2005840.
  37. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE, et al. International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2003;35(8):1381 -95. https://doi.org/10.1249/01. MSS.0000078924.61453.FB.
  38. Jackson PA, Deary ME, Reay JL, Scholey AB, Kennedy DO. No effect of 12 weeks' supplementation with 1 g DHA-rich or EPA-rich fish oil on cognitive function or mood in healthy young adults aged 18-35 years. Br J Nutr. 2012; 107(8):1232-43. https://doi.org/10.1017/S000711451100403X.
  39. Benton D, Donohoe R, Clayton D, Long S. Supplementation with DHA and the psychological functioning of young adults. Br J Nutr. 2013;109(01):155- 61. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007114512000566.
  40. Stonehouse W, Conlon CA, Podd J, Hill SR, Minihane AM, Haskell C, et al. DHA supplementation improved both memory and reaction time in healthy young adults: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2013;97(5): 1134-43. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.112.053371.
  41. Karr JE, Grindstaff TR, Alexander JE. Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and cognition in a college-aged population. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2012;20(3):236-42. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026945.
  42. Yurko-Mauro K, Alexander DD, Van Elswyk ME. Docosahexaenoic acid and adult memory: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2015;10(3): e0120391. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120391.
  43. Fontani G, Corradeschi F, Felici A, Alfatti F, Migliorini S, Lodi L. Cognitive and physiological effects of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in healthy subjects. Eur J Clin Investig. 2005;35(11):691-9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2362.2005.01570.x.
  44. Bauer I, Hughes M, Rowsell R, Cockerell R, Pipingas A, Crewther S, et al. Omega-3 supplementation improves cognition and modifies brain activation in young adults. Hum Psychopharmacol. 2014;29(2):133-44. https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.2379.
  45. Giles GE, Mahoney CR, Urry HL, Brunye TT, Taylor HA, Kanarek RB. Omega-3 fatty acids and stress-induced changes to mood and cognition in healthy individuals. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2015;132:10-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2015.02.018.
  46. Jensen MMST, Hoy CE. Correlation between level of (n-3) polyunsaturated fatty acids in brain phospholipids and learning ability in rats. Biochim Biophys Acta. 1996;1300:203-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/ 0005-2760(96)00007-0.
  47. Dalton A, Wolmarans P, Witthuhn RC, van Stuijvenberg ME, Swanevelder SA, Smuts CM. A randomised control trial in schoolchildren showed improvement in cognitive function after consuming a bread spread, containing fish flour from a marine source. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2009;80(2-3):143-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plefa.2008.12.006.
  48. Coley N, Raman R, Donohue MC, Aisen PS, Vellas B, Andrieu S. Defining the optimal target population for trials of polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation using the erythrocyte omega-3 index: a step towards personalized prevention of cognitive decline? J Nutr Health Aging. 2018;22: 982-8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-018-1052-2.
  49. Weissberger GH, Nation DA, Nguyen CP, Bondi MW, Han SD. Meta-analysis of cognitive ability differences by apolipoprotein e genotype in young humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;94:49-58. https//doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.009.
  50. Kuperstein F, Eilam R, Yavin E. Altered expression of key dopaminergic regulatory proteins in the postnatal brain following perinatal n-3 fatty acid dietary deficiency. J Neurochem. 2008;106(2):662-71. https://doi.org/10. 1111/j.1471-4159.2008.05418.x.
  51. Kitajka K, Sinclair AJ, Weisinger RS, Weisinger HS, Mathai M, Jayasooriya AP, et al. Effects of dietary omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids on brain gene expression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004;101(30):10931-6. https://doi.org/ 10.1073/pnas.0402342101.
  52. Rojas CV, Greiner RS, Fuenzalida LC, Martinez JI, Salem N Jr, Uauy R. Longterm n-3 FA deficiency modifies peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor ß mRNA abundance in rat ocular tissues. Lipids. 2002;37(4):367-74. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s1145-002-0904-4.
  53. Dennis NA, Browndyke JN, Stokes J, Need A, Burke JR, Welsh-Bohmer KA, et al. Temporal lobe functional activity and connectivity in young adult APOE E4 carriers. Alzheimers Dement. 2010;6(4):303-11. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.jalz.2009.07.003.
  54. Mondadori CR, de Quervain DJ, Buchmann A, Mustovic H, Wollmer MA, Schmidt CF, et al. Better memory and neural efficiency in young apolipoprotein E epsilon4 carriers. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(8):1934-47. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl103.
  55. Diaz-Gerevini GT, Dain A, Pasqualini ME, Lopez CB, Eynard AR, Repossi G. Diabetic encephalopathy: beneficial effects of supplementation with fatty acids w3 and nordihydroguaiaretic acid in a spontaneous diabetes rat model. Lipids Health Dis. 2019;18(1):43. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12944-018-0938-7.
  56. Calder PC. Mechanisms of action of (n-3) fatty acids. J Nutr. 2012;142(3): 592S-9S. https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.1 1 1.155259.
  57. Micallef MA, Munro IA, Garg ML. An inverse relationship between plasma n- 3 fatty acids and C-reactive protein in healthy individuals. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2009;63:1154-6. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2009.20.
  58. Chalon S. Omega-3 fatty acids and monoamine neurotransmission. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids. 2006;75(4):259-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plefa.2006.07.005.
  59. Nieoullon A. Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Prog Neurobiol. 2002;67(1):53-83. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0082(02)00011-4.
  60. Chung WL, Chen JJ, Su HM. Fish oil supplementation of control and (n-3) fatty acid-deficient male rats enhances reference and working memory performance and increases brain regional docosahexaenoic acid levels. J Nutr. 2008;138(6):1165-71. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/138.6.1165.
  61. Gamoh S, Hashimoto MM, Yanagimoto K, Katakura M, Abdul HM, Shido O. Krill-derived phospholipids rich in n-3 fatty acid improve spatial memory in adult rats. J Agric Sci. 2011;3(4):3-12. https://doi.org/10.5539/jas.v3n4p3.


Created by admin. Last Modification: Sunday November 10, 2019 02:10:06 GMT-0000 by admin. (Version 10)

Attached files

ID Name Comment Uploaded Size Downloads
12944 Omega-3 T1.jpg admin 10 Nov, 2019 90.57 Kb 406
12943 Omega-3 index cognition.jpg admin 09 Nov, 2019 34.72 Kb 352
12942 Omega-3 Index.pdf admin 09 Nov, 2019 969.20 Kb 464